The Times Australia
The Times World News

.
Times Media

.

Why do animals living with humans evolve such similar features? A new theory could explain 'domestication syndrome'

  • Written by Ben Thomas Gleeson, Doctoral Candidate, Australian National University
Why do animals living with humans evolve such similar features? A new theory could explain 'domestication syndrome'

In the 19th century, Charles Darwin was one of the first to notice something interesting about domesticated animals: different species often developed similar changes when compared to their ancient wild ancestors.

But why would a host of seemingly unrelated features repeatedly occur together in different domesticated animals?

Scientists call this collection of shared changes “domestication syndrome[1]”, and the reason it occurs is still hotly debated[2].

In a new paper in Proceedings of the Royal Society B[3], we argue that currently popular explanations aren’t quite right – and propose a new explanation focused on big changes in the way domesticated animals live. Along the way, our theory also offers insights into the unexpected story of how we humans domesticated ourselves.

Shared changes under domestication

The most commonly shared change is tamer behaviour. All domesticated animals are calmer than their wild ancestors naturally were.

That’s probably not very surprising. Ancient humans would’ve preferred docile animals, and likely selected breeding stock for tameness.

Read more: Why so many domesticated mammals have floppy ears[4]

But other common changes don’t seem at all useful to humans – or to the animals themselves. Like shorter faces, smaller teeth, more fragile skeletons, smaller brains, and different colours in skin, fur, and feathers.

Not all domesticated animals[5] share all these features. For example, dogs have many, and camels only a few.

But each change occurs in more than one domesticated species.

Wild self-domestication

Surprisingly, very similar changes sometimes also appear in wild animals, leading some scientists to think they “self-domesticated” in some way.

The bonobo (a great ape closely related to the chimpanzee) is one famous example[6] of an animal that has undergone these changes without human intervention. Urban foxes[7] are another.

Bonobos are a species who are believed to have ‘self-domesticated’. Shutterstock

Wild self-domestication is most common in isolated sub-populations, like on islands[8], and may overlap with a similar phenomenon known as the “island effect”.

Perhaps more surprisingly, modern humans also show features of domestication syndrome, when compared to our ancient ancestors. This suggests we also self-domesticated[9].

Some scientists argue these changes made us more sociable, helping us to develop complex languages and culture.

So a clearer understanding of domestication syndrome in animals might improve our knowledge of human evolution too.

What causes domestication syndrome?

In recent years, two main possible explanations for domestication syndrome have dominated scientific discussion.

The first suggests it was caused when ancient humans selected animals for tamer behaviour, which somehow triggered all of the other traits too.

This idea is supported by a famous long-running Russian fox-breeding experiment[10] which began in 1959, in which caged foxes were selected only for tameness but developed the other “unselected” features as well.

The second hypothesis[11] complements this first one. It suggests selection for tameness causes the other features because they’re all linked by genes controlling “neural crest cells[12]”. These cells, found in embryos, form many animal features – so changing them could cause several differences at once.

More than selection for tameness

However, our new research[13] suggests these two ideas oversimplify and obscure the complex evolutionary effects at play.

For one thing, there are problems with the famous Russian fox experiment. As other authors have noted, the experiment didn’t begin by taming wild foxes[14], but used foxes from a farm in Canada. And these pre-farmed foxes already had features of domestication syndrome.

What’s more, the experimenters didn’t only select for tameness. They bred other foxes for aggression, but the aggressive foxes also developed domestication syndrome features.

And in a similar experiment conducted in the 1930s[15], caged rats developed the same common changes, including tamer behaviour, despite no deliberate selection for tameness, or aggression.

So, it seems domestication syndrome might not be caused by humans selecting animals for tameness. Instead, it might be caused by unintended shared effects from the new domestic environment.

A new hypothesis for domestication syndrome

Crucially, it’s not just new forces of selection, such as a human preference for tameness, that matters. The removal of pre-existing selection is just as important, because that’s what naturally shaped the wild ancestors in the first place.

For example, domesticated animals are often protected from predators, so wild traits for avoiding them might be lost. Competition for mating partners is also often reduced, so wild reproductive features and behaviours could decline, or disappear.

Domesticated animals are also usually reliably fed. This might alter certain features, but would certainly change natural metabolism and growth.

Caged rats have also been seen to develop signs of domestication syndrome. Oxana Golubets / Unsplash

In effect, we argue there are multiple selective changes at work on domesticated animals, not just “selection for tameness”, and that shared shifts in evolutionary selection would often cause shared changes in features. Even across different species.

Our new hypothesis highlights four ways that selection shaping wild animals is often disrupted by domestication. These are:

  1. less fighting between males
  2. fewer males for females to choose between
  3. more reliable food and fewer predators, and
  4. elevated maternal stress, which initially reduces the health and survival of offspring.

Several of these might resemble “selection for tameness”, but using this one term to describe them all is misleadingly vague, and obscures other changes in selection.

So how did we domesticate ourselves?

Well, one current theory[16] is that sociable “beta males” began cooperating to kill alpha bullies. This changed how competition worked among males, leading to fewer big and aggressive males.

But our hypothesis suggests other effects also played a role. For example, our early ancestors evolved the capacity for shared infant care[17]. In our chimpanzee relatives today, sharing care of an infant would likely trigger extreme stress for the mother – but our ancestors adapted to this increased stress and gained an effective survival strategy.

Adapting to the increased maternal stress that accompanies separation from infants (either for shared care or domestication) may be one of the drivers of ‘domestication syndrome’. Shutterstock

More reliable food access due to group foraging and sharing, plus collective defence against predators, might also have made us more sociable, more cooperative, and more complex, while promoting other changes commonly seen in non-human domesticated animals.

Whatever the specific drivers in each species, recognising multiple selective pathways better explains the domestication syndrome, and reaffirms the complexity of evolutionary effects shaping all life on Earth.

References

  1. ^ domestication syndrome (theconversation.com)
  2. ^ hotly debated (www.cell.com)
  3. ^ a new paper in Proceedings of the Royal Society B (doi.org)
  4. ^ Why so many domesticated mammals have floppy ears (theconversation.com)
  5. ^ Not all domesticated animals (royalsocietypublishing.org)
  6. ^ one famous example (www.scientificamerican.com)
  7. ^ Urban foxes (www.science.org)
  8. ^ like on islands (pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov)
  9. ^ we also self-domesticated (www.youtube.com)
  10. ^ long-running Russian fox-breeding experiment (evolution-outreach.biomedcentral.com)
  11. ^ second hypothesis (theconversation.com)
  12. ^ neural crest cells (www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov)
  13. ^ new research (doi.org)
  14. ^ didn’t begin by taming wild foxes (www.nytimes.com)
  15. ^ a similar experiment conducted in the 1930s (babel.hathitrust.org)
  16. ^ one current theory (www.cambridge.org)
  17. ^ capacity for shared infant care (www.youtube.com)

Read more https://theconversation.com/why-do-animals-living-with-humans-evolve-such-similar-features-a-new-theory-could-explain-domestication-syndrome-201765

The Times Features

HCF’s Healthy Hearts Roadshow Wraps Up 2024 with a Final Regional Sprint

Next week marks the final leg of the HCF Healthy Hearts Roadshow for 2024, bringing free heart health checks to some of NSW’s most vibrant regional communities. As Australia’s ...

The Budget-Friendly Traveler: How Off-Airport Car Hire Can Save You Money

When planning a trip, transportation is one of the most crucial considerations. For many, the go-to option is renting a car at the airport for convenience. But what if we told ...

Air is an overlooked source of nutrients – evidence shows we can inhale some vitamins

You know that feeling you get when you take a breath of fresh air in nature? There may be more to it than a simple lack of pollution. When we think of nutrients, we think of t...

FedEx Australia Announces Christmas Shipping Cut-Off Dates To Help Beat the Holiday Rush

With Christmas just around the corner, FedEx is advising Australian shoppers to get their presents sorted early to ensure they arrive on time for the big day. FedEx has reveale...

Will the Wage Price Index growth ease financial pressure for households?

The Wage Price Index’s quarterly increase of 0.8% has been met with mixed reactions. While Australian wages continue to increase, it was the smallest increase in two and a half...

Back-to-School Worries? 70% of Parents Fear Their Kids Aren’t Ready for Day On

Australian parents find themselves confronting a key decision: should they hold back their child on the age border for another year before starting school? Recent research from...

Times Magazine

How Does a Legal Separation Differ from a Divorce

A divorce is not an easy choice to make. A divorce formally terminates the marriage, but a legal separation is a court order that dictates the couple's rights and responsibilities while they remain married but live apart. Both agreements offer le...

Fukushima Treated Water Release: Skepticism Of Environmental Organizations And Green Parties Contrary To Japan’s Decommissioning Efforts

Since it officially announced its plan to release the treated water from the Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station (FDNPS), Japan has been targeted by multiple actors. Despite the overwhelming support from legitimate international organizatio...

Take Advantage of Cloud Accounting Software to Unlock Maximum Efficiency

In today's fast-paced business environment, it's critical to have access to real-time financial information. A cloud accounting solution provides a cost-effective, secure, and efficient way to manage your business's financial activities, regardless...

Australia Post - Christmas International sending dates fast approaching

Australia Post has today announced the need-to-know dates for more than 180 international destinations to help Australians sending Christmas cards and presents to loved ones overseas. For Economy Air, many destinations require cards and gifts to b...

Asia’s Finest: The Prophets of Football from the East

The culture of Asiatic countries isn’t that prone to football, or at least to the practice or affiliation to clubs in general. They prize, probably due to the irrelevance of the Asiatic teams in the wide world of football, a bigger appreciation f...

The right stuff: 5 essential data discovery traits to look out for

It is a business-intelligence system that allows companies to receive detailed, highly interactive information from a variety of data sources. As opposed to standard business-intelligence, data mining focuses on big picture trends with a less str...